In 1962, a cave explorer named Michel Siffre
started a series of experiments where he isolated himself underground for months
without light or clocks.
He attached himself to electrodes that monitored his vital signs
and kept track of when he slept and ate.
When Siffre finally emerged,
the results of his pioneering experiments
revealed that his body had kept to a regular sleeping-waking cycle.
Despite having no external cues,
he fell asleep,
woke up,
and ate at fixed intervals.
This became known as a circadian rhythm from the Latin for “about a day.”
Scientists later found these rhythms affect our hormone secretion,
how our bodies process food,
and even the effects of drugs on our bodies.
The field of sciences studying these changes is called chronobiology.
Being able to sense time helps us do everything from waking and sleeping
to knowing precisely when to catch a ball that’s hurtling towards us.
We owe all these abilities to an interconnected system of timekeepers
in our brains.
It contains the equivalent of a stopwatch telling us how many seconds elapsed,
a clock counting the hours of the day,
and a calendar notifying us of the seasons.
Each one is located in a different brain region.
Siffre, stuck in his dark cave, relied on the most primitive clock
in the suprachiasmatic nucleus, or SCN of the hypothalamus.
Here’s the basics of how we think it works based on fruitfly and mouse studies.
Proteins known as CLK, or clock, accumulate in the SCN throughout the day.
In addition to activating genes that tell us to stay awake,
they make another protein called PER.
When enough PER accumulates,
it deactivates the gene that makes CLK,
eventually making us fall asleep.
Then, clock falls low, so PER concentrations also drop again,
allowing CLK to rise,
starting the cycle over.
There are other proteins involved,
but our day and night cycle may be driven in part by this seesaw effect
between CLK by day and PER by night.
For more precision,
our SCNs also rely on external cues
like light,
food,
noise,
and temperature.
We called these zeitgebers,
German for “givers of time.”
Siffre lacked many of these cues underground,
but in normal life, they fine tune our daily behavior.
For instance, as natural morning light filters into our eyes,
it helps wake us up.
Traveling through the optic nerve to the SCN,
it communicates what’s happening in the outside world.
The hypothalamus then halts the production of melatonin,
a hormone that triggers sleep.
At the same time,
it increases the production of vasopressin
and noradrenaline throughout the brain,
which help control our sleep cycles.
At about 10 am,
the body’s rising temperature drives up our energy and alertness,
and later in the afternoon,
it also improves our muscle activity and coordination.
Bright screens at night can confuse these signals,
which is why binging on TV before bed makes it harder to sleep.
But sometimes we need to be even more precise when telling the time,
which is where the brain’s internal stopwatch chimes in.
One theory for how this works involves the fact
that communication between a given pair of neurons
always takes roughly the same amount of time.
So neurons in our cortex and other brain areas
may communicate in scheduled, predictable loops
that the cortex uses to judge with precision how much time has passed.
That creates our perception of time.
In his cave, Siffre made a fascinating additional discovery about this.
Every day, he challenged himself to count up to 120
at the rate of one digit per second.
Over time, instead of taking two minutes, it began taking him as long as five.
Life in the lonely, dark cave had warped Siffre’s own perception of time
despite his brain’s best efforts to keep him on track.
This makes us wonder what else influences our sense of time.
And if time isn’t objective, what does that mean?
Could each of us be experiencing it differently?
Only time will tell.